No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Conciliation and meta-contrast are important for understanding how people assign group memberships during conflict situations
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 07 July 2022
Abstract
Pietraszewski misrepresents both the nature of behaviour in conflict and the ability of psychology to theorise the relational properties of group designation. At the behavioural level, he focusses exclusively on “attack,” when consolation/care in conflict is equally present and important. At the theoretical level, he ignores existing psychological work on how group perception is shaped by the meta-contrast principle.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64(2), 123–152. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043805CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Waal, F. B. (2000). Primates – A natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science, 289(5479), 586–590. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.289.5479.586CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ejbye-Ernst, P., Lindegaard, M. R., & Bernasco, W. (in press). A CCTV-based analysis of target selection by guardians intervening in interpersonal conflicts. European Journal of Criminology, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477370820960338Google Scholar
Haslam, S. A., Reicher, S. D., & Levine, M. (2012). When other people are heaven, when other people are hell: How social identity determines the nature and impact of social support. In Jetten, J., Haslam, C., & Haslam, S. A. (Eds.), The social cure: Identity, health, and well being (pp. 157–174). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Levine, M., Taylor, P. J., & Best, R. (2011). Third parties, violence, and conflict resolution: The role of group size and collective action in the microregulation of violence. Psychological Science, 22(3), 406–412. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611398495CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebst, L. S., Philpot, R., Bernasco, W., Dausel, K. L., Ejbye-Ernst, P., Nicolaisen, M. H., & Lindegaard, M. R. (2019). Social relations and presence of others predict bystander intervention: Evidence from violent incidents captured on CCTV. Aggressive Behavior, 45(6), 598–609. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21853CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liebst, L. S., Philpot, R., Levine, M., & Lindegaard, M. R. (2021). Cross-national CCTV footage shows low victimization risk for bystander interveners in public conflicts. Psychology of Violence, 11(1), 11–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philpot, R. (2017). Beyond the dyad: The role of groups and third-parties in the trajectory of violence. Open Research Exeter, University of Exeter.Google Scholar
Philpot, R., Liebst, L. S., Levine, M., Bernasco, W., & Lindegaard, M. R. (2020a). Would I be helped? Cross-national CCTV footage shows that intervention is the norm in public conflicts. American Psychologist, 75(1), 66–75. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philpot, R., Liebst, L. S., Lindegaard, M. R., Verbeek, P., & Levine, M. (2020b). Reconciliation in human adults: A video-assisted naturalistic observational study of post conflict conciliatory behaviour in interpersonal aggression. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/9e4rfGoogle Scholar
Reicher, S. (2004). The context of social identity: Domination, resistance, and change. Political Psychology, 25(6), 921–945. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00403.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salzarulo, L. (2004). Formalizing self-categorization theory to simulate the formation of social groups. In Hernández, C., López-Paredes, A., Pajares, J., & Galán, J. M. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference of European Social Simulation Association. University of Valladolid.Google Scholar
Salzarulo, L. (2006). A continuous opinion dynamics model based on the principle of meta-contrast. Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, 9(1), 1–13.Google Scholar
Smith, J. R., & Hogg, M. A. (2008). Social identity and attitudes. In Crano, W. D., & Prislin, R. (Eds.), Attitudes and attitude change (pp. 337–360). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory (pp. x, 239). Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Target article
Toward a computational theory of social groups: A finite set of cognitive primitives for representing any and all social groups in the context of conflict
Related commentaries (29)
A neuroscientific perspective on the computational theory of social groups
Advantages and limitations of representing groups in terms of recursive utilities
Are we there yet? Every computational theory needs a few black boxes, including theories about groups
Beyond folk-sociology: Extending Pietraszewski's model to large-group dynamics
Can group representations based on relational cues warrant the rich inferences typically drawn from group membership?
Coalitionary psychology and group dynamics on social media
Compassion within conflict: Toward a computational theory of social groups informed by maternal brain physiology
Conciliation and meta-contrast are important for understanding how people assign group memberships during conflict situations
Developmental antecedents of representing “group” behavior: A commentary on Pietraszewski's theory of groups
Group? What group? A computational model of the group needs a psychology of “us” (not “them”)
How do we know who may replace each other in triadic conflict roles?
Interacting with others while reacting to the environment
Internal versus external group conflicts
Latent structure learning as an alternative computation for group inference
Learning agents that acquire representations of social groups
More than one way to skin a cat: Addressing the arbitration problem in developmental science
On vagueness and parochialism in psychological research on groups
Paranoia reveals the complexity in assigning individuals to groups on the basis of inferred intentions
Private versus public: A dual model for resource-constrained conflict representations
Psychological and actual group formation: Conflict is neither necessary nor sufficient
Shadow banning, astroturfing, catfishing, and other online conflicts where beliefs about group membership diverge
Shared intentionality and the representation of groups; or, how to build a socially adept robot
Signals and cues of social groups
Social groups and the computational conundrums of delays, proximity, and loyalty
Societies and other kinds of social groups
The labelled container: Conceptual development of social group representations
Towards a computational network theory of social groups
Triadic conflict “primitives” can be reduced to welfare trade-off ratios
Using laboratory intergroup conflict and riots as a “stress test”
Author response
More “us,” less “them”: An appeal for pluralism – and stand-alone computational theorizing – in our science of social groups