Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T05:28:54.289Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A mature second-person neuroscience needs a first-person (plural) developmental foundation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 July 2013

Charlie Lewis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 YF, United Kingdom. c.lewis@lancaster.ac.ukhttp://www.psych.lancs.ac.uk/people/charlie-lewis
James Stack
Affiliation:
Department of Education, Liverpool Hope University, Hope Park, Liverpool L16 9JD, United Kingdom. stackj@hope.ac.uk

Abstract

Schilbach et al.'s model assumes that the ability to “experience” minds is already present in human infants and therefore falls foul of the very intellectualist problems it attempts to avoid. We propose an alternative relational, action-based account, which attempts to grasp how the individual's construction of knowledge develops within interactions.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, J. W. P. & Bickhard, M. H. (2013) Stepping off the pendulum: Why only an action-based approach can transcend the nativist-empiricist debate. Cognitive Development 28:96133.Google Scholar
Barresi, J. & Moore, C. (1996) Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19(1):107–22. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00041790.Google Scholar
Carpendale, J. E. M. & Lewis, C. (2004) Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children's social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(1):79151.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Shipstead, S. G. & Croft, K. (1980) What young children think you see when their eyes are closed. Cognition 8:369–87.Google Scholar
Hurley, S. (2008) The shared circuits model (SCM): How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31(1):158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuigan, N. & Doherty, M. J. (2006) Head and shoulders, knees and toes: Which parts of the body are necessary to be seen? British Journal of Developmental Psychology 24:727–32.Google Scholar
Moll, H., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2011) Social engagement leads 2-year-olds to overestimate others' knowledge. Infancy 16:248–65.Google Scholar
O'Neill, D. (1996) Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests. Child Development 67:659–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onishi, K. H. & Baillargeon, R. (2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308:255–58.Google Scholar
Sodian, B. & Thoermer, C. (2008) Precursors to a Theory of Mind in infancy: Perspectives for research on autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61:2739.Google Scholar
Stack, J. & Lewis, C. (2008) Steering towards a developmental account of infant social understanding. Human Development 51:229–34.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (1999) Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and nonpurposeful behavior. Infant Behavior and Development 22:145–60.Google Scholar