Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T17:23:09.876Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Second-person social neuroscience: Connections to past and future theories, methods, and findings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 July 2013

Nicolas Vermeulen
Affiliation:
Psychological Sciences Research Institute (IPSY), Université Catholique de Louvain (UCL), 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium. Nicolas.Vermeulen@uclouvain.behttp://uclouvain.academia.edu/NicolasVermeulenGordy.Pleyers@uclouvain.behttp://www.ecsa.ucl.ac.be/pleyers National Fund for Scientific Research (FRS-FNRS), B-1000 Brussels, Belgium
Gordy Pleyers
Affiliation:
Psychological Sciences Research Institute (IPSY), Université Catholique de Louvain (UCL), 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium. Nicolas.Vermeulen@uclouvain.behttp://uclouvain.academia.edu/NicolasVermeulenGordy.Pleyers@uclouvain.behttp://www.ecsa.ucl.ac.be/pleyers
Martial Mermillod
Affiliation:
UniversitéGrenoble Alpes, LPNC, F-38040, Grenoble & CNRS, LPNC UMR 5105, F-38040 Grenoble, France. Martial.Mermillod@upmf-grenoble.frhttp://webu2.upmf-grenoble.fr/lpnc/membre_martial_mermillod Institut Universitaire de France, 75005 Paris, France

Abstract

We argue that Schilbach et al. have neglected an important part of the social neuroscience literature involving participants in social interactions. We also clarify some part of the models the authors discussed superficially. We finally propose that social neuroscience should take into consideration the effect of being observed and the complexity of the task as potentially influencing factors.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Cheetham, M., Pedroni, A. F., Antley, A., Slater, M. & Jäncke, L. (2009) Virtual Milgram: Empathic concern or personal distress? Evidence from functional MRI and dispositional measures. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 29:113. Available at: http://www.frontiersin.org/Human_Neuroscience/10.3389/neuro.09.029.2009/full Google Scholar
Dindo, M., Whiten, A. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2009) Social facilitation of exploratory foraging behavior in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). American Journal of Primatology 71:419–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I. & Lieberman, M. D. (2004) Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(7):294300.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D. & Williams, K. D. (2003) Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302(5643):290–92.Google Scholar
Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C. D. & Singer, T. (2010) Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members' suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron 68:149–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitayama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R. & Suzuki, T. (2004) Is there any “free” choice? Self and dissonance in two cultures. Psychological Science 15:527–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maurage, P., Joassin, F., Philippot, P., Heeren, A., Vermeulen, N., Mahau, P., Delperdande, C., Corneille, O., Luminet, O. & De Timary, P. (2012) Disrupted regulation of social exclusion in alcohol-dependence: An fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 37:2067–75. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2012.54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, D., Atzeni, T. & Butera, F. (2004) Coaction and upward social comparison reduce the illusory conjunction effect: Support for distraction-conflict theory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 40:659–65.Google Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M. & Hess, U. (2010) The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(6):417–33; discussion 433–80. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000865.Google Scholar
Olsson, A., Ebert, J. P., Banaji, M. R. & Phelps, E. A. (2005) The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science 309:785–87.Google Scholar
Rossignol, M., Anselme, C., Vermeulen, N., Philippot, P. & Campanella, S. (2007) Categorical perception of anger and disgust facial expression is affected by non-clinical social anxiety: An ERP study. Brain Research 1132(1):166–76.Google Scholar
Strauss, B. (2001) Social facilitation in motor tasks: A review of research and theory. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 3:237–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vermeulen, N. & Luminet, O. (2009) Alexithymia factors and memory performances for neutral and emotional words. Personality and Individual Differences 47:305309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vermeulen, N., Luminet, O., de Sousa, M. C. & Campanella, S. (2008) Categorical perception of anger is disrupted in alexithymia: Evidence from a visual ERP study. Cognition and Emotion 22(6):1052–67.Google Scholar
Vermeulen, N., Toussaint, J. & Luminet, O. (2010) The influence of alexithymia and music on the incidental memory for emotion words. European Journal of Personality 24(6):551–68.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1965) Social facilitation. Science 149(3681):269–74.Google Scholar