Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by Crossref.
Froese, Tom
Gershenson, Carlos
and
Rosenblueth, David A.
2013.
The dynamically extended mind.
p.
1419.
Koike, Takahiko
Tanabe, Hiroki C.
and
Sadato, Norihiro
2015.
Hyperscanning neuroimaging technique to reveal the “two-in-one” system in social interactions.
Neuroscience Research,
Vol. 90,
Issue. ,
p.
25.
Wiltshire, Travis J.
Lobato, Emilio J. C.
McConnell, Daniel S.
and
Fiore, Stephen M.
2015.
Prospects for direct social perception: a multi-theoretical integration to further the science of social cognition.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
Vol. 8,
Issue. ,
Zapata-Fonseca, Leonardo
Dotov, Dobromir
Fossion, Ruben
and
Froese, Tom
2016.
Time-Series Analysis of Embodied Interaction: Movement Variability and Complexity Matching As Dyadic Properties.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 7,
Issue. ,
Young, Gerald
2016.
Unifying Causality and Psychology.
p.
93.
Froese, Tom
and
Zapata-Fonseca, Leonardo
2017.
Commentary: Alignment in social interactions.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 8,
Issue. ,
2019.
ハイパースキャニング脳機能イメージング.
Journal of the Society of Biomechanisms,
Vol. 43,
Issue. 3,
p.
179.
Abramova, Ekaterina
and
Slors, Marc
2019.
Mechanistic explanation for enactive sociality.
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences,
Vol. 18,
Issue. 2,
p.
401.
Candadai, Madhavun
Setzler, Matt
Izquierdo, Eduardo J.
and
Froese, Tom
2019.
Embodied Dyadic Interaction Increases Complexity of Neural Dynamics: A Minimal Agent-Based Simulation Model.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 10,
Issue. ,
Wang, Chenbo
Zhang, Tingyu
Shan, Zhoukuidong
Liu, Jieqiong
Yuan, Di
and
Li, Xianchun
2019.
Dynamic interpersonal neural synchronization underlying pain‐induced cooperation in females.
Human Brain Mapping,
Vol. 40,
Issue. 11,
p.
3222.
Bolis, Dimitris
and
Schilbach, Leonhard
2020.
“Through others we become ourselves”: The dialectics of predictive coding and active inference.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Vol. 43,
Issue. ,
Resendiz-Benhumea, Georgina Montserrat
Sangati, Ekaterina
and
Froese, Tom
2020.
Levels of Coupling in Dyadic Interaction: An Analysis of Neural and Behavioral Complexity.
p.
2250.
Martens, Judith H.
2021.
Habit and Skill in the Domain of Joint Action.
Topoi,
Vol. 40,
Issue. 3,
p.
663.
Loh, Chen Lam
and
Froese, Tom
2021.
An Oscillator Model for Interbrain Synchrony: Slow Interactional Rhythms Entrain Fast Neural Activity.
p.
1.
Lehmann, Konrad
Bolis, Dimitris
Friston, Karl J.
Schilbach, Leonhard
Ramstead, Maxwell J. D.
and
Kanske, Philipp
2023.
An Active-Inference Approach to Second-Person Neuroscience.
Perspectives on Psychological Science,
Hermans, Karlijn S. F. M.
Kirtley, Olivia J.
Kasanova, Zuzana
Achterhof, Robin
Hagemann, Noëmi
Hiekkaranta, Anu P.
Lecei, Aleksandra
Zapata-Fonseca, Leonardo
Lafit, Ginette
Fossion, Ruben
Froese, Tom
and
Myin-Germeys, Inez
2023.
Ecological and Convergent Validity of Experimentally and Dynamically Assessed Capacity for Social Contingency Detection Using the Perceptual Crossing Experiment in Adolescence.
Assessment,
Vol. 30,
Issue. 4,
p.
1109.
Target article
Toward a second-person neuroscience1
Related commentaries (28)
A mature second-person neuroscience needs a first-person (plural) developmental foundation
A second-person approach cannot explain intentionality in social understanding
Advancing the neuroscience of social emotions with social immersion
Brain games: Toward a neuroecology of social behavior
From synthetic modeling of social interaction to dynamic theories of brain–body–environment–body–brain systems
From the bottom up: The roots of social neuroscience at risk of running dry?
Further steps toward a second-person neuroscience
Interaction versus observation: A finer look at this distinction and its importance to autism
It takes two to talk: A second-person neuroscience approach to language learning
Merging second-person and first-person neuroscience
Mirror neurons are central for a second-person neuroscience: Insights from developmental studies
On projecting grammatical persons into social neurocognition: A view from linguistics
Parameterising ecological validity and integrating individual differences within second-person neuroscience
Reciprocity between second-person neuroscience and cognitive robotics
Second person neuroscience needs theories as well as methods
Second-person neuroscience: Implications for Wittgensteinian and Vygotskyan approaches to psychology
Second-person social neuroscience: Connections to past and future theories, methods, and findings
Social affordances in context: What is it that we are bodily responsive to?
Social affordances: Is the mirror neuron system involved?
Social cognition is not a special case, and the dark matter is more extensive than recognized
Social perception and “spectator theories” of other minds
Talking to each other and talking together: Joint language tasks and degrees of interactivity
The brain as part of an enactive system
The second person in “I”-“you”-“it” triadic interactions
The use of non-interactive scenarios in social neuroscience
Toward a neuroscience of interactive parent–infant dyad empathy
What we can learn from second animal neuroscience
Why not the first-person plural in social cognition?
Author response
A second-person neuroscience in interaction1