No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Predictability or controllability: Which matters more for the BCD?
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 29 November 2017
Abstract
Pepper & Nettle's theory of the behavioral constellation of deprivation (BCD) would benefit from teasing apart the conceptually distinct – although related – constructs of predictability and control. Our commentary draws from prior research conducted in the learning domain to demonstrate that predictability moderates the effects of control and independently exerts a powerful influence on outcomes relevant to the BCD.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
Badia, P., Harsh, J. & Abbott, B. (1979) Choosing between predictable and unpredictable shock conditions: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin
86(5):1107–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dess, N. K., Linwick, D., Patterson, J., Overmier, J. B. & Levine, S. (1983) Immediate and proactive effects of controllability and predictability on plasma cortisol responses to shocks in dogs. Behavioral Neuroscience
97(6):1005–16.Google Scholar
Fokidis, H. B., des Roziers, M. B., Sparr, R., Rogowski, C., Sweazea, K. & Deviche, P. (2012) Unpredictable food availability induces metabolic and hormonal changes independent of food intake in a sedentary songbird. Journal of Experimental Biology
215(16):2920–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galhardo, L., Vital, J. & Oliveira, R. F. (2011) The role of predictability in the stress response of a cichlid fish. Physiology & Behavior
102(3):367–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glass, D. C. & Singer, J. E. (1973) Experimental studies of uncontrollable and unpredictable noise. Representative Research in Social Psychology
4(1):165–83.Google Scholar
Grillon, C., Baas, J. M., Cornwell, B. & Johnson, L. (2006) Context conditioning and behavioral avoidance in a virtual reality environment: Effect of predictability. Biological Psychiatry
60(7):752–59.Google Scholar
Herry, C., Bach, D. R., Esposito, F., Di Salle, F., Perrig, W. J., Scheffler, K., Lüthi, A. & Seifritz, E. (2007) Processing of temporal unpredictability in human and animal amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience
27(22):5958–66.Google Scholar
Kidd, C., Palmeri, H. & Aslin, R. N. (2013) Rational snacking: Young children's decision-making on the marshmallow task is moderated by beliefs about environmental reliability. Cognition
126(1):109–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koolhaas, J. M., Bartolomucci, A., Buwalda, B., de Boer, S. F., Flügge, G., Korte, S. M., Meerlo, P., Murison, R., Olivier, B., Palanza, P., Richter-Levin, G., Sgoifo, A., Steimer, T., Stiedl, O., van Dijk, G., Wöhr, M. & Fuchs, E. (2011) Stress revisited: A critical evaluation of the stress concept. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews
35(5):1291–301.Google Scholar
LaDage, L. D. (2015) Environmental change, the stress response, and neurogenesis. Integrative and Comparative Biology
55(3):372–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martf, O. & Armario, A. (1997) Influence of regularity of exposure to chronic stress on the pattern of habituation of pituitary-adrenal hormones, prolactin and glucose. Stress
1(3):179–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mineka, S. & Hendersen, R. W. (1985) Controllability and predictability in acquired motivation. Annual Review of Psychology
36(1):495–529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mittal, C. & Griskevicius, V. (2014) Sense of control under uncertainty depends on people's childhood environment: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
107(4):621–37.Google Scholar
Nickels, J. B., Cramer, K. M. & Gural, D. M. (1992) Toward unconfounding prediction and control: Predictionless control made possible. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement
24(2):156–70.Google Scholar
Overmier, J. B. & Wielkiewicz, R. M. (1983) On unpredictability as a causal factor in “learned helplessness.” Learning and Motivation
14(3):324–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, L. T. & Hill, E. M. (2002) Childhood unpredictability, schemas for unpredictability, and risk taking. Social Behavior and Personality
30(5):453–73.Google Scholar
Schmitz, A., Merikangas, K., Swendsen, H., Cui, L., Heaton, L. & Grillon, C. (2011) Measuring anxious responses to predictable and unpredictable threat in children and adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
110(2):159–70.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. & Binik, Y. M. (1977): The safety signal hypothesis. In: Operant-Pavlovian interactions, ed. Davis, H. & Hurwitz, H. M. B., pp. 165–87. Hillsdale.Google Scholar
Waitt, C. & Buchanan-Smith, H. M. (2001) What time is feeding? How delays and anticipation of feeding schedules affect stump-tailed macaque behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science
75(1):75–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, J. M. (1971) Effects of coping behavior in different warning signal conditions on stress pathology in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology
77(1):1–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wieser, M. J., Reicherts, P., Juravle, G. & von Leupoldt, A. (2016) Attention mechanisms during predictable and unpredictable threat – A steady-state visual evoked potential approach. NeuroImage
139:167–75.Google Scholar
Wortman, C. B. (1975) Some determinants of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
31(2):282–94.Google Scholar
Target article
The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences
Related commentaries (31)
Beyond personal control: The role of developing self-control abilities in the behavioral constellation of deprivation
Both collection risk and waiting costs give rise to the behavioral constellation of deprivation
Cultural consonance, deprivation, and psychological responses for niche construction
Deprived, but not depraved: Prosocial behavior is an adaptive response to lower socioeconomic status
Developing the behavioural constellation of deprivation: Relationships, emotions, and not quite being in the present
Divergent life histories and other ecological adaptations: Examples of social-class differences in attention, cognition, and attunement to others
Epigenetic-based hormesis and age-dependent altruism: Additions to the behavioural constellation of deprivation
Evolutionary approaches to deprivation transform the ethics of policy making
From perceived control to self-control, the importance of cognitive and emotional resources
Health behaviour, extrinsic risks, and the exceptions to the rule
Intergenerational capital flows are central to fitness dynamics and adaptive evolution in humans
Interpreting risky behavior as a contextually appropriate response: Significance and policy implications beyond socioeconomic status
Intertemporal impulsivity can also arise from persistent failure of long-term plans
It's not just about the future: The present payoffs to behaviour vary in degree and kind between the rich and the poor
Loss of control is not necessary to induce behavioral consequences of deprivation: The case of religious fasting during Ramadan
Predictability or controllability: Which matters more for the BCD?
Public health interventions can increase objective and perceived control by supporting people to enact the choices they want to make
Relative state, social comparison reactions, and the behavioral constellation of deprivation
Socioeconomic status, unpredictability, and different perceptions of the same risk
Stuff goes wrong, so act now
The behavioral constellation of deprivation may be best understood as risk management
The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Compelling framework, messy reality
The elusive constellations of poverty
The link between deprivation and its behavioural constellation is confounded by genetic factors
The physiological constellation of deprivation: Immunological strategies and health outcomes
The uncontrollable nature of early learning experiences
The “appropriate” response to deprivation: Evolutionary and ethical dimensions
Toward a balanced view of stress-adapted cognition
Uncertainty about future payoffs makes impatience rational
What about the behavioral constellation of advantage?
When does deprivation motivate future-oriented thinking? The case of climate change
Author response
Strengths, altered investment, risk management, and other elaborations on the behavioural constellation of deprivation