Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T15:52:01.219Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cultural consonance, deprivation, and psychological responses for niche construction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 November 2017

Robert J. Quinlan*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164-4910. rquinlan@wsu.eduhttp://public.wsu.edu/~rquinlan/

Abstract

Cultural consonance is a measure of culturally encoded goals relevant to psychological, behavioral, and health responses to deprivation. Similar to extrinsic mortality, low cultural consonance and an associated inability to predict adaptive outcomes may activate impulsivity, delay discounting, and reward seeking. Low cultural consonance could promote “fast life history” in low-quality environments and motivate cultural niche construction for local adaptation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barkow, J. H. (1989) Darwin, sex, and status: Biological approaches to mind and culture. University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Borgerhoff Mulder, B., Bowles, S., Hertz, T., Bell, A., Beise, J., Clark, G., Fazzio, I., Gurven, M., Hill, K., Hooper, P. L., Irons, W., Kaplan, H., Leonetti, D., Low, B., Marlowe, F., McElreath, R., Naidu, S., Nolin, D., Piraino, P., Quinlan, R., Schniter, E., Sear, R., Shenk, M., Smith, E. A., von Rueden, C. & Wiessner, P. (2009) Intergenerational wealth transmission and the dynamics of inequality in small-scale societies. Science 326(5953):682–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S. & White, T. L. (1994) Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67(2):319–33.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2013) Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(3):181204.Google Scholar
Deck, C. & Jahedi, S. (2015) The effect of cognitive load on economic decision making: A survey and new experiments. European Economic Review 78:97119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. W. (2012) Cultural consonance: Linking culture, the individual and health. Preventive Medicine 55(5):390–93.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. W., Balieiro, M. C., de Araújo, L. F., Silva, W. A. Jr. & dos Santos, J. E. (2016) Culture as a mediator of gene-environment interaction: Cultural consonance, childhood adversity, a 2A serotonin receptor polymorphism, and depression in urban Brazil. Social Science and Medicine 161:109–17.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. W., Balieiro, M. C. & dos Santos, J. E. (2017) Cultural consonance in life goals and depressive symptoms in urban Brazil. Journal of Anthropological Research 73(1):4365.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. W., Balieiro, M. C., Ribeiro, R. P. & dos Santos, J. E. (2007a) A prospective study of cultural consonance and depressive symptoms in urban Brazil. Social Science & Medicine 65(10):2058–69.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. W., Balieiro, M. C., Ribeiro, R. P. & dos Santos, J. E. (2007b) Cultural consonance and psychological distress: Examining the associations in multiple cultural domains. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 31(2):195224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dressler, W. W., Borges, C. D., Balieiro, M. C. & dos Santos, J. E. (2005) Measuring cultural consonance: Examples with special reference to measurement theory in anthropology. Field Methods 17(4):331–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. W., Ribeiro, R. P., Balieiro, M. C., Oths, K. S. & dos Santos, J. E. (2004) Eating, drinking and being depressed: The social, cultural and psychological context of alcohol consumption and nutrition in a Brazilian community. Social Science and Medicine 59(4):709–20.Google Scholar
Friston, K., Adams, R. A., Perrinet, L. & Breakspear, M. (2012) Perceptions as hypotheses: Saccades as experiments. Frontiers in Psychology 3(151):120.Google Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2016) The extended evolutionary synthesis, ethnography, and the human niche: Toward an integrated anthropology. Current Anthropology 57(S13):1326.Google Scholar
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W. & Robertson, T. E. (2011b) The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 100(6):1015–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L. & Whitney, P. (2003) Impulsive decision making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology 29(2):298306.Google ScholarPubMed
Irons, W. (1998) Adaptively relevant environments versus the environment of evolutionary adaptedness. Evolutionary Anthropology 6(6):194204.Google Scholar
Kendal, J., Jamshid, J., Tehrani, J. J. & Odling-Smee, J. (2011) Human niche construction in interdisciplinary focus. Philosophical Trans of the Royal Society 366(1566):785–92.Google Scholar
Koffarnus, M. N., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T. & Bickel, W. K. (2013) Changing delay discounting in the light of the competing neurobehavioral decision systems theory: A review. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 99(1):3257.Google Scholar
Macfarlan, S. J., Remiker, M. & Quinlan, R. J. (2012) Competitive altruism explains labor exchange variation in a Dominican community. Current Anthropology 53(3):118–24.Google Scholar
Macfarlan, S. J., Walker, R. S., Flinn, M. V. & Chagnon, N. A. (2014) Lethal coalitionary aggression and long-term alliance formation among Yanomamö men. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111(47):16662–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattison, S. M. (2011) Evolutionary contributions to solving the “matrilineal puzzle”: A test of Holden, Sear, and Mace's model. Human Nature 22 (1–2):6488.Google Scholar
Morean, M. E., DeMartini, K. S., Leeman, R. F., Pearlson, G. D., Anticevic, A., Krishnan-Sarin, S., Krystal, J. H. & O'Malley, S. (2014) Psychometrically improved, abbreviated version of three classic measures of impulsivity and self-control. Psychological Assessment 26(3):1003–20.Google Scholar
Quinlan, R. J., Jilo Dira, S., Caudell, M. & Quinlan, M. B. (2016) Culture and psychological responses to environmental shocks: Cultural ecology of Sidama impulsivity and niche construction in SW Ethiopia. Current Anthropology 57(5):632–52.Google Scholar
Reyes-García, V., Gravlee, C. C., McDade, T. W., Huanca, T., Leonard, W. R. & Tanner, S. (2010) Cultural consonance and psychological well-being. Estimates using longitudinal data from an Amazonian society. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry 34(1):186203.Google Scholar
Rickard, I. J., Frankenhuis, W. E. & Nettle, D. (2014) Why are childhood family factors associated with timing of maturation? A role for internal prediction. Perspectives on Psychological Science 9(1):315.Google Scholar
Roepstorff, A., Niewöhner, J. & Beck, S. (2010) Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks 23 (8–9):1051–59.Google Scholar
Romney, A. K., Batchelder, W. H. & Weller, S. C. (1987) Recent applications of cultural consensus theory. American Behavioral Scientist 31(2):163–77.Google Scholar
Stotz, K. (2010) Human nature and cognitive–developmental niche construction. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9(4):483501.Google Scholar
Strauss, C. & Quinn, N. (1997) A cognitive theory of cultural meaning. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stulp, G., Sear, R. & Barrett, L. (2016) The reproductive ecology of industrial societies, part I: Why measuring fertility matters. Human Nature 27(4):422–44.Google Scholar
Toussaint, M. (2009) Probabilistic inference as a model of planned behavior. Künstliche Intelligenz. Available at: http://ipvs.informatik.uni-stuttgart.de/mlr/marc/publications/09-toussaint-KI.pdf.Google Scholar
Weller, S. (2007) Cultural consensus theory: Applications and frequently asked questions. Field Methods 19:339–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar