No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
The elusive constellations of poverty
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 29 November 2017
Abstract
Pepper & Nettle describe possible processes underlying what they call a behavioral constellation of deprivation (BCD). Although we are certain about the application of evolutionary models to our understanding of poverty, we are less certain about the utility of behavioral constellations. The empirical record on poverty-related behaviors is much more divergent and broad than such constellations suggest.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
Banerjee, A. V. & Duflo, E. (2011) Poor economics: A radical rethinking of the way to fight global poverty. PublicAffairs.Google Scholar
Bertrand, M., Mullainathan, S. & Shafir, E. (2004) A behavioral-economics view of poverty. American Economic Review
94(2):419–23.Google Scholar
Callan, M. J., Kim, H., Gheorghiu, A. I. & Matthews, W. J. (2016) The interrelations between social class, personal relative deprivation, and prosociality. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1–10. Available at: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/1948550616673877.Google Scholar
Carvalho, L. S., Meier, S. & Wang, S. W. (2016) Poverty and economic decision-making: Evidence from changes in financial resources at payday. American Economic Review
106(2):260–84.Google Scholar
Duncan, G., Magnuson, K. & Votruba-Drzal, E. (2017) Moving beyond correlations in assessing the consequences of poverty. Annual Review of Psychology
68:413–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griskevicius, V., Ackerman, J., Cantú, S., Delton, A., Robertson, T., Simpson, J., Thompson, M. & Tybur, J. (2013) When the economy falters, do people spend or save? Responses to resource scarcity depend on childhood environments. Psychological Science
24(2):197–205.Google Scholar
Hamamura, T. (2012) Social class predicts generalized trust but only in wealthy societies. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
43(3):498–509.Google Scholar
Haushofer, J. & Fehr, E. (2014) On the psychology of poverty. Science
344(6186):862–67.Google Scholar
Kraus, M. W., Piff, P. K., Mendoza-Denton, R., Rheinschmidt, M. L. & Keltner, D. (2012) Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: How the rich are different from the poor. Psychological Review
119(3):546–72.Google Scholar
Lewis, O. (1966) La vida: A Puerto Rican family in the culture of poverty—San Juan and New York. Random House.Google Scholar
Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E. & Zhao, J. (2013) Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science
341(6149):976–80.Google Scholar
Mullainathan, S. & Shafir, E. (2013) Scarcity: Why having too little means so much. Times Books, Macmillan and Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Pampel, F. C., Krueger, P. & Denney, J. (2010) Socioeconomic disparities in health behaviors. Annual Review of Sociology
36:349–70.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K., Shafir, E. & Mullainathan, S. (2015) Scarcity frames value. Psychological Science
26(4):402–12.Google Scholar
Target article
The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences
Related commentaries (31)
Beyond personal control: The role of developing self-control abilities in the behavioral constellation of deprivation
Both collection risk and waiting costs give rise to the behavioral constellation of deprivation
Cultural consonance, deprivation, and psychological responses for niche construction
Deprived, but not depraved: Prosocial behavior is an adaptive response to lower socioeconomic status
Developing the behavioural constellation of deprivation: Relationships, emotions, and not quite being in the present
Divergent life histories and other ecological adaptations: Examples of social-class differences in attention, cognition, and attunement to others
Epigenetic-based hormesis and age-dependent altruism: Additions to the behavioural constellation of deprivation
Evolutionary approaches to deprivation transform the ethics of policy making
From perceived control to self-control, the importance of cognitive and emotional resources
Health behaviour, extrinsic risks, and the exceptions to the rule
Intergenerational capital flows are central to fitness dynamics and adaptive evolution in humans
Interpreting risky behavior as a contextually appropriate response: Significance and policy implications beyond socioeconomic status
Intertemporal impulsivity can also arise from persistent failure of long-term plans
It's not just about the future: The present payoffs to behaviour vary in degree and kind between the rich and the poor
Loss of control is not necessary to induce behavioral consequences of deprivation: The case of religious fasting during Ramadan
Predictability or controllability: Which matters more for the BCD?
Public health interventions can increase objective and perceived control by supporting people to enact the choices they want to make
Relative state, social comparison reactions, and the behavioral constellation of deprivation
Socioeconomic status, unpredictability, and different perceptions of the same risk
Stuff goes wrong, so act now
The behavioral constellation of deprivation may be best understood as risk management
The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Compelling framework, messy reality
The elusive constellations of poverty
The link between deprivation and its behavioural constellation is confounded by genetic factors
The physiological constellation of deprivation: Immunological strategies and health outcomes
The uncontrollable nature of early learning experiences
The “appropriate” response to deprivation: Evolutionary and ethical dimensions
Toward a balanced view of stress-adapted cognition
Uncertainty about future payoffs makes impatience rational
What about the behavioral constellation of advantage?
When does deprivation motivate future-oriented thinking? The case of climate change
Author response
Strengths, altered investment, risk management, and other elaborations on the behavioural constellation of deprivation