No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Humans are not the Borg: Personal and social selves function as components in a unified self-system
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 26 October 2016
Abstract
Does joining groups trigger a cascade of psychological processes that can result in a loss of individuality and lead to such outcomes as social loafing and poor decision-making? Rather than privileging the self comprising primarily individual qualities as the “true self,” a multilevel, multicomponent approach suggests that, in most cases, personal and collective identities are integrated and mutually sustaining.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016
References
Allport, F. H. (1962) A structuronomic conception of behavior: Individual and collective. I. Structural theory and the master problem of social psychology. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology
64:3–30. doi: 10.1037/h0043563.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (2012) Optimal distinctiveness theory: Its history and development. In: Handbook of theories of social psychology, vol. 2, ed. Van Lange, P. A. M., Kruglanski, A. W. & Higgins, E. T., pp. 81–98. Sage.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B. & Chen, Y.-R. (2007) Where (who) are collectives in collectivism? Toward conceptual clarification of individualism and collectivism. Psychological Review
114:133–51.Google Scholar
Bromley, D. G. (2001) A tale of two theories: Brainwashing and conversion as competing political narratives. In: Misunderstanding cults, ed. Zablocki, B. & Robbins, T., pp. 318–48. University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Bunderson, J. S. & Boumgarden, P. (2010) Structure and learning in self-managed teams: Why “bureaucratic” teams can be better learners. Organization Science
21(3):609–24.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1922) Group psychology and the analysis of the ego (trans. Strachey, J.). Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psycho-analysis.Google Scholar
Harkins, S. G. (1987) Social loafing and social facilitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
23(1):1–18.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A. (2005b) The social identity perspective. In: The handbook of group research and practice, ed. Wheelan, S., pp. 133–57. Sage.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A., Terry, D. J. & White, K. M. (1995) A tale of two theories: A critical comparison of identity theory with social identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly
58(4):255–69.Google Scholar
Kozlowski, S. W. J. & Bell, B. S. (2013) Work groups and teams in organizations: Review update. In Handbook of psychology: Vol. 12. Industrial and organizational psychology, second edition, ed. Schmitt, N. & Highhouse, S., pp. 412–69. Wiley.Google Scholar
Larson, J. R. Jr. (2010) In search of synergy in small group performance. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Le Bon, G. (1896/1960) The crowd: A study of the popular mind [La psychologie des foules]. Viking Press. (Original work published in 1896.)Google Scholar
Mulder, M. B., Bowles, S., Hertz, T., Bell, A., Beise, J., Clark, G., Fazzio, I., Gurven, M., Hill, K., Hooper, P. L., Irons, W., Kaplan, H., Leonetti, D., Low, B., Marlowe, F., McElreath, R., Naidu, S., Nolin, D., Piraino, P., Quinlan, R., Schniter, E., Sear, R., Shenk, M., Smith, E. A., von Rueden, C. & Wiessner, P. (2009) Intergenerational wealth transmission and the dynamics of inequality in small-scale societies. Science
326(5953):682–88.Google Scholar
Postmes, T. & Spears, R. (1998) Deindividuation and antinormative behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin
123:238–59. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.123.3.238.Google Scholar
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., Luke, M. A., O'Mara, E. M. & Gebauer, J. E. (2013) A three-tier hierarchy of self-potency: Individual self, relational self, collective self. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology
48(1):235–95.Google Scholar
Stets, J. E. & Burke, P. J. (2000) Identity theory and social identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly
63:224–37.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Gómez, Á., Huici, C., Morales, J. F. & Hixon, J. G. (2010) Identity fusion and self-sacrifice: Arousal as a catalyst of pro-group fighting, dying, and helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
99(5):824–41.Google Scholar
Tuckman, B. (1965) Developmental sequence in small groups. Psychological Bulletin
63:384–99. doi: 10.1037/h0022100.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B., Heingartner, A. & Herman, E. M. (1969) Social enhancement and impairment of performance in the cockroach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
13:83–92.Google Scholar
Target article
Are groups more or less than the sum of their members? The moderating role of individual identification
Related commentaries (29)
Beyond old dichotomies: Individual differentiation can occur through group commitment, not despite it
But is it social? How to tell when groups are more than the sum of their members
Considering the role of ecology on individual differentiation
Differentiated selves can surely be good for the group, but let's get clear about why
Differentiated selves help only when identification is strong and tasks are complex
Differentiation of selves: Differentiating a fuzzy concept
Disputing deindividuation: Why negative group behaviours derive from group norms, not group immersion
Group and individual as complementary conceptual categories
Group behavior in the military may provide a unique case
Group effort in resuscitation teams
Group members differ in relative prototypicality: Effects on the individual and the group
Group membership: Who gets to decide?
Groups need selves, but which selves? Dual selves in groups and the downsides of individuation
How group members contribute to group performance: Evidence from agent-based simulations
Humans are not the Borg: Personal and social selves function as components in a unified self-system
Identity matters to individuals: Group assessment cannot be reduced to collective performance
Member differentiation and group tasks: More than meets the eye
Not even wrong: Imprecision perpetuates the illusion of understanding at the cost of actual understanding
Reputational concerns as a general determinant of group functioning
Roles and ranks: The importance of hierarchy for group functioning
Social identification is generally a prerequisite for group success and does not preclude intragroup differentiation
Social, not individual, identification is the key to understanding group phenomena
Solved paradoxes and old hats? The research needed on differentiated selves
Task specificity and the impact on both the individual and group during the formation of groups
The hows and whys of “we” (and “I”) in groups
The subtle effects of incentives and competition on group performance
The unique role of the agent within the romantic group
Vicarious contagion decreases differentiation – and comes with costs
We agree and we disagree, which is exactly what most people do most of the time
Author response
Differentiating selves facilitates group outcomes