No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Timers from birth: Early timing abilities exceed limits of the temporal updating system
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 12 December 2019
Abstract
Hoerl & McCormack argue that children are incapable of reasoning about time until age 5. However, their dual timing perspective does not address non-symbolic timing, or timing in the absence of symbols/language. Given substantial evidence that infants and children are capable of non-symbolic timing, I argue that infants and children are well-tuned timers prior to age 5.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019
References
Brannon, E. M., Libertus, M. E., Meck, W. H. & Woldorff, M. G. (2008) Electrophysiological measures of time processing in infant and adult brains: Weber's Law holds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20(2):193–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brannon, E. M., Roussel, L. W., Meck, W. H. & Woldorff, M. (2004) Timing in the baby brain. Cognitive Brain Research 21(2):227–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brannon, E. M., Suanda, S. & Libertus, K. (2007) Temporal discrimination increases in precision over development and parallels the development of numerosity discrimination. Developmental Science 10(6):770–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Church, R. M. & Deluty, M. Z. (1977) Bisection of temporal intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 3(3):216.Google ScholarPubMed
Droit-Volet, S., Clement, A. & Wearden, J. (2001) Temporal generalization in 3-to 8-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 80(3):271–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Droit-Volet, S. & Wearden, J. H. (2001) Temporal bisection in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 80(2):142–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbon, J. (1977) Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. Psychological Review 84(3):279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamamouche, K. & Cordes, S. (2019). Learning about time: Knowledge of formal timing symbols is related to individual differences in temporal precision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meck, W. H. & Church, R. M. (1983) A mode control model of counting and timing processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 9(3):320.Google ScholarPubMed
Odic, D. (2018) Children's intuitive sense of number develops independently of their perception of area, density, length, and time. Developmental Science 21(2):e12533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odic, D., Lisboa, J. V., Eisinger, R., Olivera, M. G., Maiche, A. & Halberda, J. (2016) Approximate number and approximate time discrimination each correlate with school math abilities in young children. Acta Psychologica 163:17–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Provasi, J., Rattat, A. C. & Droit-Volet, S. (2011) Temporal bisection in 4-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 37(1):108.Google ScholarPubMed
vanMarle, K. & Wynn, K. (2006) Six-month-old infants use analog magnitudes to represent duration. Developmental Science 9(5):F41–F49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
Thinking in and about time: A dual systems perspective on temporal cognition
Related commentaries (33)
A dual-systems perspective on temporal cognition: Implications for the role of emotion
A theory stuck in evolutionary and historical time
Animals are not cognitively stuck in time
Are counterfactuals in and about time?
Beings in the moment
Closing the symbolic reference gap to support flexible reasoning about the passage of time
Dual systems for all: Higher-order, role-based relational reasoning as a uniquely derived feature of human cognition
From temporal updating to temporal reasoning: Developments in young children's temporal representations
Future-oriented objects
Identity-based motivation and the paradox of the future self: Getting going requires thinking about time (later) in time (now)
Let's call a memory a memory, but what kind?
Limitations of Hoerl and McCormack's dual systems model of temporal consciousness
Locating animals with respect to landmarks in space-time
Locating the contradiction in our understanding of time
Neural correlates of temporal updating and reasoning in association with neuropsychiatric disorders
No doing without time
Nonhuman sequence learning findings argue against Hoerl and McCormack's two systems of temporal cognition
On believing that time does not flow, but thinking that it seems to
On the human uniqueness of the temporal reasoning system
Problems with the dual-systems approach to temporal cognition
Temporal representation and reasoning in non-human animals
Temporal updating, behavioral learning, and the phenomenology of time-consciousness
The dual systems in temporal cognition: A spatial analogy
The “now moment” is believed privileged because “now” is when happening is experienced
Thinking about the past as the past for the past's sake: Why did temporal reasoning evolve?
Thinking about thinking about time
Thinking about time and number: An application of the dual-systems approach to numerical cognition
Time, flow, and space
Timers from birth: Early timing abilities exceed limits of the temporal updating system
Two challenges for a dual system approach to temporal cognition
Updating and reasoning: Different processes, different models, different functions
Updating the dual systems model of temporal cognition: Reasoning with dynamic systems theory
What time words teach us about children's acquisition of the temporal reasoning system
Author response
Temporal updating, temporal reasoning, and the domain of time