Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T14:38:38.406Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Paranoia reveals the complexity in assigning individuals to groups on the basis of inferred intentions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 July 2022

Anna Greenburgh
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, LondonWC1H 0AP, UK a.greenburgh@ucl.ac.uk n.raihani@ucl.ac.uk
Nichola Raihani
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, LondonWC1H 0AP, UK a.greenburgh@ucl.ac.uk n.raihani@ucl.ac.uk

Abstract

We suggest that variation, error, and bias will be essential to include in a complete computational theory of groups – particularly given that formation of group representations must often rely on inferences of intentions. We draw on the case study of paranoia to illustrate that intentions that do not correspond to group-constitutive roles may often be perceived as such.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bebbington, P. E., McBride, O., Steel, C., Kuipers, E., Radovanoviĉ, M., Brugha, T., … Freeman, D. (2013). The structure of paranoia in the general population. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 202(6), 419427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, N. (1959). The paranoid pseudo-community revisited. American Journal of Sociology, 65(1), 5258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Uscinski, J. E., Sutton, R. M., Cichocka, A., Nefes, T., Ang, C. S., & Deravi, F. (2019). Understanding conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 40, 335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., & Bentall, R. P. (2017). The concomitants of conspiracy concerns. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 52(5), 595604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D., Loe, B. S., Kingdon, D., Startup, H., Molodynski, A., Rosebrock, L., … Bird, J. C. (2021). The revised Green et al., Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS): Psychometric properties, severity ranges, and clinical cut-offs. Psychological Medicine, 51(2), 244253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D., Waite, F., Rosebrock, L., Petit, A., Causier, C., East, A., … Lambe, S. (2020). Coronavirus conspiracy beliefs, mistrust, and compliance with government guidelines in England. Psychological Medicine, 52(2), 113. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720001890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenburgh, A., Bell, V., & Raihani, N. (2019). Paranoia and conspiracy: Group cohesion increases harmful intent attribution in the trust game. PeerJ, 7, e7403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, C., Garety, P. A., Freeman, D., Fowler, D., Bebbington, P., Dunn, G., & Kuipers, E. (2006). Content and affect in persecutory delusions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 561577. doi: 10.1348/014466506X98768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imhoff, R., & Lamberty, P. (2018). How paranoid are conspiracy believers? Toward a more fine-grained understanding of the connect and disconnect between paranoia and belief in conspiracy theories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(7), 909926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raihani, N. J., & Bell, V. (2017). Paranoia and the social representation of others: A large-scale game theory approach. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raihani, N. J., & Bell, V. (2019). An evolutionary perspective on paranoia. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(2), 114121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saalfeld, V., Ramadan, Z., Bell, V., & Raihani, N. J. (2018). Experimentally induced social threat increases paranoid thinking. Royal Society Open Science, 5(8), 180569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed