Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T15:21:16.302Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The integrated information theory of consciousness: Unmasked and identified

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 March 2022

Bjorn Merker
Affiliation:
Independent Scholar, Fjälkestadsv. 410-82, 29194Kristianstad, Swedenbjornmerker@gmail.com
Kenneth Williford
Affiliation:
Department of Philosophy & Humanities, University of Texas at Arlington, Arlington, TX76019, USAwilliford@uta.eduhttps://mentis.uta.edu/explore/profile/kenneth-williford
David Rudrauf
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Education Science, Swiss Center for Affective Science, University Center of Computer Science, University of Geneva, Geneva, 1202, Switzerland. David.Rudrauf@unige.chhttps://www.unige.ch/fapse/mmef/en/recherche/

Abstract

In our response to a truly diverse set of commentaries, we first summarize the principal topical themes around which they cluster, then address two “outlier” positions (the problem of consciousness has been solved vs. is intractable). Next, we address ways in which commentaries by non-integrated information theory (IIT) authors engage with the specifics of our IIT critique, turning finally to the four commentaries by IIT authors.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baars, B. J. (1993). How does a serial, integrated and very limited stream of consciousness emerge from a nervous system that is mostly unconscious, distributed, parallel and of enormous capacity? In: Theoretical and experimental studies of consciousness, Ciba Foundation Symposium 174, ed. G. Bock & J. Marsh, pp. 282–290. Wiley.Google Scholar
Bayne, T. (2018). On the axiomatic foundations of the integrated information theory of consciousness. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 4(1), 18 niy007. doi: 10.1093/nc/niy007Google Scholar
Boccaletti, S., Latora, V., Moreno, Y., Chavez, M., & Hwang, D.-U. (2006). Complex networks: Structure and dynamics. Physics Reports, 424, 175308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physrep.2005.10.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, G.-N. (2002). Fundamental algorithms of space-variant vision: Non-uniform sampling, triangulation, and foveal scale-space. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University Graduate School of Arts and Sciences.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1978). Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. The MIT Press, Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Gattass, R., Nascimento-Silva, S., Soares, J. G. M., Lima, B., Karla Jansen, A., Diogo, A. C. M., … Fiorani, M. (2005). Cortical visual areas in monkeys: Location, topography, connections, columns, plasticity and cortical dynamics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 360, 709731. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1629CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haun, A., & Tononi, G. (2019). Why does space feel the way it does? Towards a principled account of spatial experience. Entropy, 21(12), 1160 https://doi.org/10.3390/e21121160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kihlstrom, J. (1996) Perception without awareness of what is perceived, learning without awareness of what is learned. In Velmans, M. (Ed.), The science of consciousness: Psychological, neuropsychological and clinical reviews (pp. 2346). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, C. (2019). The feeling of life itself: Why consciousness is widespread but can't be computed. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolster, H., Peeters, R., & Orban, G. A. (2010). The retinotopic organization of the human middle temporal area MT/V5 and its cortical neighbors. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 98019820. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2069-10.2010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandler, G. A. (1975). Consciousness: Respectable, useful, and probably necessary. In: Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium, ed. R. Solso, pp. 229–254. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. A. (2002). Consciousness recovered. Psychological functions and origins of conscious thought. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McFarland, D. J., & Sibly, R. M. (1975). The behavioural final common path. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society (London), 270, 265293.Google ScholarPubMed
Mediano, P. A. M., Seth, A. K., & Barrett, A. B. (2019b). Measuring integrated information: Comparison of candidate measures in theory and simulation. Entropy, 21, 17 (1–30). doi: 10.3390/e21010017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merker, B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 63134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milgram, S. (1967). The small world problem. Psychology Today, 2, 6067.Google Scholar
Morsella, E., Velasquez, A. G., Yankulova, J. K., Li, Y., Wong, C. Y. & Lambert, D. (2020). Motor cognition: The role of sentience in perception and action. Kinesiology Review, 9, 261274. https://doi.org/10.1123/kr.2020-0017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oizumi, M., Albantakis, L., & Tononi, G. (2014). From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated information theory 3.0. PLoS Computational Biology, 10, e1003588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudrauf, D., Bennequin, D., Granic, I., Landini, G., Friston, K., & Williford, K. (2017). A mathematical model of embodied consciousness. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 428, 106131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2017.05.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudrauf, D., Bennequin, D., & Williford, K. (2020). The moon illusion explained by the projective consciousness model. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 507, 110455 (1–20). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2020.110455CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudrauf, D., Sergeant-Perthuis, G., Belli, O., Tisserand, Y., & Di Marzo Serugendo, G. (2022). Modeling the subjective perspective of consciousness and its role in the control of behaviours. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 534, 133, Article 110957. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2021.110957CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, E. (1980). Computational anatomy and functional architecture of striate cortex: A spatial mapping approach to perceptual coding. Vision Research, 20, 645669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, E. L. (1977). Spatial mapping in primate sensory projection: Analytic structure and relevance to perception. Biological Cybernetics, 25, 181194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tononi, G., & Edelman, G. M. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282, 18461851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velmans, M. (1991). Is human information processing conscious? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 651726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williford, K., Bennequin, D., & Rudrauf, D. (under review). Pre-reflective self-consciousness and projective geometry.Google Scholar